Applicability of an immersive virtual reality system for assessing route learning in older adults
Aplicabilidade de um sistema de realidade virtual imersivo para avaliação da aprendizagem de rotas em idosos
INTRODUCTION
Topographical orientation, or spatial orientation, is the ability to find oneâ?Ts way around an environment, follow familiar routes, recognize places, and learn new routes, and it is an essential skill for a personâ?Ts autonomy
1,2. The main spatial orientation strategies are egocentric and allocentric
3.
Along with these strategies, the individual needs to remember a series of motion directions at decision points to walk on a path at a greater distance than can be viewed in a single time, in other words, the direction of the destination
1,4. A sense of direction leads to learning a new route, where the subject, within the environment itself, incorporates information through repeated visualizations of the environment and continuous changes in the egocentric orientation, being able to, when walking this path again, go from your origin to your destination
1.
Spatial disorientation is a common finding in Alzheimerâ?Ts disease (AD), even in its early stages and, since the neurodegenerative process of AD precedes the clinical signs for diagnosis, it is believed that the detection of spatial disorientation in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a predictor of higher risk of conversion of these patients
5. Thus, the assessment of spatial orientation associated with other clinical assessments in patients with MCI can be a cognitive marker of AD
6,7.
There is no consensus on the best way to assess spatial orientation, and the traditionally used pen and paper tests are not sufficiently sensitive and ecological to detect spatial disorientation
6. An assessment is considered ecological when it investigates the patientâ?Ts skills and difficulties as close as possible to their reality, exposing them to their daily life problems
8. Non-ecological assessments do not adequately evaluate the impairment of spatial orientation experienced by the patient in the real world
9.
New computerized methods have been developed to assess spatial orientation
10,11. The use of virtual reality (VR) allows the patient to interact in environments like real ones through projection in three-dimensional (3D) scenarios and the use of oneâ?Ts sensory channels to interact with the visual and auditory stimuli of virtual systems
6,12,13. The sense of presence and immersion experienced by the patient in an immersive virtual environment is greater, and the individual can interact with its elements, favoring their behavior as if they were acting in the real world
14,15. Thus, immersive VR could simulate more realistic environments when compared to paper and pencil assessments and, therefore, more accurately reproduce the difficulties of spatial orientation and identify more subtle deficits
15,16.
With the lack of a reference standard to assess spatial orientation and the advantages of evaluating this cognitive domain through ecological tasks, our research group developed a system called Spatial Orientation in Immersive Virtual Environment Test (SOIVET)
7,17. This system contains a task called Route (SOIVET-Route), which focuses on route learning.
In 2021, Costa et al.
18 analyzed the concurrent validity of the SOIVET-Route, concluding that it is a valid tool for assessing the spatial orientation of older adults, but its applicability and stability have not yet been analyzed. Therefore, this study aimed to compare the applicability of the SOIVET-Route in older adults with and without MCI. Besides, we assessed the short- and long-term stability of its assessments.
METHODS
Study sample
This study was conducted by following the guidelines and regulatory standards for research involving human beings (resolution 466/12 of the National Health Council), consubstantiated opinion no. 2.580.187, with the Certificate of Presentation for Ethical Appreciation (CAAE) no. 84904018.6.0000.0065, approved by the Ethics and Research Committee of the Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo.
Data collection started in February 2019 and ended in March 2020 involving the participation of 45 older adults: 22 were elderly residents in the community without objective evidence of cognitive impairment verified through a screening test â?" the Addenbrookeâ?Ts Cognitive Examination â?" Revised (ACE-R)
19 and 23 elderly individuals with a diagnosis of MCI referred from the Hospital das ClÃnicas of the Faculdade de Medicina of the Universidade de São Paulo.
Eligibility criteria included age ≥60 years; absence of a history of vertigo and/or labyrinthopathy; normal or corrected visual and auditory acuity; written informed consent form (ICF) for participation in the study; a score of >82 on the ACE-R for the control group; and a diagnosis of MCI according to Petersenâ?Ts criteria
20 for the MCI group. Participants with incapacity to understand the instructions and interact with the tasks were excluded. In addition, participants who presented any limiting adverse events during the experience with the immersive system were excluded. Finally, older adults who abandoned the study before completing all its stages were excluded (dropouts).
Data collection
The project was presented to the participants who met the eligibility criteria of the study. After agreeing to participate and signing the ICF, the participants were assessed in two different moments with an interval of 7â?"14 days between the sessions.
In the first moment, data collection started with the application of the ACE-R. Later, the sociodemographic characterization, motion sickness screening, and technology use profile questionnaires were applied.
After completing the questionnaires, the participants were guided to perform the tasks. Finally, immediately after completing the VR task, the Witmer and Singer Presence Questionnaire
21 and the questionnaire to identify adverse symptoms were applied.
In the second moment, only the task in a virtual environment was reapplied.
Research procedures
Sample characterization, cognitive screening, and motion sickness screening
A sociodemographic questionnaire and a questionnaire describing familiarization with the use of technology, both developed by the study authors, were applied to characterize the sample
7. Scoring on the technology use profile questionnaire varies from 0 to 40 points; with the higher the score, the greater the familiarity of participants with technology.
The ACE-R was applied for cognitive screening. Individuals with ACE-R score of >82 were included in the group of older adults without cognitive impairment. This score is considered a cutoff value for older adults with cognitive impairment without dementia
22.
The absence of a history of vertigo and labyrinthopathy was assessed through self-report. Participants were also assessed using a motion sickness screening questionnaire, prepared by the study authors, with scores ranging from 0 to 6 points, with the higher the score, the greater the discomfort prior to task performance.
SOIVET-Route task
The task performed is an adaptation of the Route subitem of the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT)
23. The test can be conducted in a wide variety of locations and does not establish a minimum or maximum distance between them.
In this study, we used the main entrance of the Hospital das ClÃnicas of the Faculdade de Medicina of the Universidade de São Paulo (ICHC-FMUSP) as a reference for the development of the task in a virtual environment, using similar dimensions to simulate the virtual environment task as realistically as possible, and it occurred in the following four stages:
The avatar took the route together with the participant stopping at five different places with time to observe the surroundings. The stop points were the reception, a newsstand outside the building, a cafeteria, a table, and the entrance to the study center in this order (Figure 1).
After completing the route with the avatar, the participants were invited to retrace the route alone stopping at the same places (immediate recall task).
After 20 min, the individual was asked to recreate the route once again (late recall task).
The participants were invited to return to the data collection place between 7 and 14 days to perform the abovementioned items 1â?"3 (Figure 2).
Figure 1. Route in the real and virtual tasks (aerial view of the
Figure 2. Reproduction of the real environment in virtual
The system was developed using the Oculus Rift
® head-mounted display and its controller.
The system automatically extracts the number of locations visited in the correct (correct) or incorrect (errors) sequence. The score, which can vary from 0Â to 5 points, is assigned according to the participantâ?Ts performance and the number of correct answers (places visited in the correct sequence).
Assessment of the system applicability in older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment
Applicability was evaluated based on the report of adverse symptoms triggered by the task, such as general discomfort, headache, nausea, pallor, vomiting, sweating, and fatigue. The occurrence of these symptoms was assessed through a self-report questionnaire in which the higher the score, the lower the tolerability of the task performed, with a maximum score of 64 points
7.
The sense of presence was assessed using the Witmer and Singer Presence Questionnaire
21. This questionnaire aims to measure the degree that individuals experience immersion in an environment, depending on the attentional resources existing in the environment to be explored. The questionnaire comprises 22 items, totaling 154 points. In the end, the higher the score, the greater the sense of presence and immersion, that is, the task applicability.
Assessment of short- and long-term stability
Stability is the degree to which similar results are obtained at two different times, and for that, two correlations were made between the participantâ?Ts performance in the SOIVET-Route task at two different times: correlation between immediate recall and late recall (short-term stability) and correlation between the first and second test days (long-term stability).
Statistical analysis
The collected data were processed using the SPSS version 20.0 software. The participantsâ?T clinical and sociodemographic characteristics were expressed as mean, standard deviation (SD), and 95% confidence interval (95%CI) for the numerical variables, whereas the categorical variables were expressed as absolute and relative frequencies.
Short- and long-term stabilities were analyzed using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) between the first and second evaluation days and between the immediate and late recall evaluations performed on the same day using the following analysis criteria: ICC<0.4: poor, 0.4â?"6: reasonable, 0.6â?"0.75: good, and 0.75â?"1.0: excellent. We adopted an alpha of 0.05 as statistical significance
24.
The Mann-Whitney U test was applied to compare the applicability measures of the SOIVET-Route task between older adults with and without MCI. Hedgeâ?Ts g test was used to calculate the effect size considering the following results: large ≥0.8, medium 0.8â?"0.2, and small <0.2
25.
RESULTS
The mean age of participants was 71.4 years (SD=5.5), and there was a predominance of females (n=28; 62%). Most participants had completed their higher education (n=29; 65%) and reported having some comorbidity (n=33; 75%). The most common comorbidities were systemic arterial hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and osteoporosis.
Among the 45 older adults who participated in the study, 23 (51%) met MCI diagnostic criteria and the other participants presented no complaints or evidence of having cognitive impairment, which verified through higher scores on the ACE-R, with a statistically significant difference in relation to those with MCI (p<0.001) (Table 1).
Assessment of applicability
The elderly participants reported a small number of adverse events (mean=1.46; SD=2.11), with mild dizziness and nausea as the most frequent, and high levels of sense of presence and immersion (mean=138.04; SD=14.80). There was no difference between groups with and without MCI (Table 2).
However, 17% of participants were unable to complete the task due to complaints of nausea and severe dizziness: 5 (23%) elderly people without MCI and 4 (17%) with MCI. These participants were not included in the analyses, keeping 45 elderly graduating.
There was no correlation between the participantsâ?T scores in the questionnaires of adverse symptoms or sense of presence with age, gender, education, screening for motion sickness, and technology usage profile.
Assessment of short- and long-term stability
Assessment of short- and long-term stability showed an excellent correlation for short-term stability (immediate and late recall, performed with an interval of 20 min) with effect size of 80% power, but poor to reasonable correlation for long-term stability (performed with an interval of 7â?"14 days) (Table 3).
DISCUSSION
This study analyzed the applicability of the SOIVET-Route task and its respective stability in older adults with and without MCI.
For a VR assessment to be applicable in the elderly population, a low rate of adverse symptoms is necessary
15,26. In this study, the elderly participants had good tolerability to perform the task, and there were minimal reports of adverse symptoms among those who managed to complete the assessment. There was no difference between groups and the incidence of dropouts because tolerability was 17%, which is considered low compared with the rates of 30â?"80% found in the literature
27. There was also no correlation between the scores of the adverse symptomsâ?T questionnaire and age, gender, education level, or familiarity with technology. This indicates that these factors did not influence the applicability of immersive VR in the elderly population. Nevertheless, the immersive system needs to be accurate in capturing the patientâ?Ts movements and the lowest latency possible in relation to the image displacement so that there is no sensory conflict and occurrence of possible adverse events
15,27.
These findings corroborate the study conducted by Kim et al.
15, who used the Oculus Rift
® to assess elderly people with and without Parkinsonâ?Ts disease and obtained the results similar to this research. The studies that identified higher incidence rates of adverse events used immersive VR devices with lower visual processing speed, which induced delays between movement and simulation, increasing the occurrence of possible adverse symptoms.
The sense of presence experienced by the patients was another important aspect of the applicability of immersive VR. The greater the sense of presence in a VR task, the more the individuals experience actions and emotions similar to real-life situations, making it more environmentally friendly
7,28. In this study, the high sense of presence scores were verified, and there was no statistically significant difference between the groups, suggesting that the SOIVET-Route task can be an ecological task despite having fewer sensory cues compared with the same task performed in a real environment.
Regarding the short- and long-term stability of the SOIVET-Route task, the analysis showed a good to excellent correlation in short-term stability (immediate and late recall, performed with a 20-min interval) and a poor to reasonable correlation in long-term stability (between the first and second test days, performed with an interval of 7â?"14 days). Bearing in mind that the system itself provides the orientation regarding the proposed activity during the task and registers the score, no performance bias from the evaluators is expected. This difference in short- and long-term stability could be explained by improved performance as a result of practice, as the participants were repeatedly exposed to the task, especially in the group of elderly people with MCI.
Among the studies that have investigated spatial orientation in elderly people through VR
10,29-34, only Pouya et al.
32 addressed test stability. Pouya et al.
32 obtained strong correlations in their analyses, but the participants repeated the VR assessment after 6 and 12 months of the first assessment, which may have prevented a possible learning effect and/or memorization of the task because of the long gap between exposures.
To better generalize the results, considering that the elderly performed better as they were repeatedly exposed to the task, it would be interesting to have more time for them to become familiarized with the system before recording their performance. A variation in the location of the five stop points during the routeâ?"in case there was a need to reapply the task at shorter intervalsâ?"would be interesting to avoid possible route memorization.
Finally, transposition of the immersive VR task to a tablet with a rotation sensor could be an alternative for the elderly people who presented adverse symptoms in immersion. However, the task would most likely have a lower sense of presence compared with immersive VR, but it would still be advantageous for elderly people with greater sensitivity to motion sickness.
The VR system developed to assess the route learning was applicable for older adults with and without MCI. The assessments showed good short-term stability. These results encourage the use of innovative tasks and immersive virtual environments for the assessment of cognition in older adults.
Authorsâ?T contributions. MDS, JMG, RQMC and JEP: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, validation, visualization, writing â?" original draft and writing â?" review & editing; LAPV and SMDB: methodology and writing â?" review & editing; EGM: conceptualization, project administration and software; RDL: resources.
REFERENCES
1. Guariglia CC. Orientação topográfica na doença de Alzheimer. São Paulo. Dissertation [thesis]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo; 2006.
2. Brandt T, Zwergal A, Glasauer S. 3-D Spatial memory and navigation: functions and disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. 2017;30(1):90-7.
https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0000000000000415
3. Guariglia CC, Nitrini R. Topographical disorientation in Alzheimerâ?Ts disease. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2009;67(4):967-72.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000600001
4. Hartmeyer S, Grzeschik R, Wolbers T, Wiener JM. The effects of attentional engagement on route learning performance in a virtual environment: an aging study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:235.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00235
5. Gazova I, Vlcek K, Laczó J, Nedelska Z, Hyncicova E, Mokrisova I, Sheardova K, Hort J. Spatial navigation - a unique window into physiological and pathological aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2012;4:16.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2012.00016
6. Lithfous S, Dufour A, Després O. Spatial navigation in normal aging and the prodromal stage of Alzheimerâ?Ts disease: Insights from imaging and behavioral studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12(1):201-13.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2012.04.007
7. Costa RQ, Pompeu JE, Mello DD, Moretto E, Rodrigues FZ, Santos MD, et al. Two new virtual reality tasks for the assessment of spatial orientation: preliminary results of tolerability, sense of presence and usability. Dement Neuropsychol. 018;12(2):196-204.
https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-020013
8. Morganti F, Gaggioli A, Strambi L, Rusconi ML, Riva G. A virtual reality extended neuropsychological assessment for topographical disorientation: a feasibility study. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2007;4:26.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-4-26
9. Hegarty M, Montello DR, Richardson AE, Ishikawa T, Lovelace K. Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence. 2006;34(2):151-76.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005
10. Cushman LA, Duffy CJ. Detecting navigational deficits in cognitive aging and Alzheimer disease using virtual reality. Neurology. 2008;71(12):888-95.
https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000326262.67613.fe
11. Allison SL, Fagan AM, Morris JC, Head D. Spatial navigation in preclinical Alzheimerâ?Ts disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52(1):77-90.
https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150855
12. Kim J, Filho J, Ko N, Yoon B. Unsupervised virtual reality-based exercise program improves hip muscle strength and balance control in older adults: a pilot study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;94(5):937-43.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2012.12.010
13. Lin CS, Jeng MY, Yeh TM. The elderly perceived meanings and values of virtual reality leisure activities: a means-end chain approach. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):663.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040663
14. Oliveira CR, Lopes Filho BJ, Sugarman MA, Esteves CS, Lima MM, Moret-Tatay C, et al. Development and Feasibility of a Virtual Reality Task for the Cognitive Assessment of Older Adults: The ECO-VR. Span J Psychol. 2016;19:E95.
https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.96
15. Kim A, Darakjian N, Finley JM. Walking in fully immersive virtual environments: an evaluation of potential adverse effects in older adults and individuals with Parkinsonâ?Ts disease. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2017;14(1):16.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-017-0225-2
16. Davison SM, Deeprose C, Terbeck S. A comparison of immersive virtual reality with traditional neuropsychological measures in the assessment of executive functions. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2018;30(2):79-89.
https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2017.14
17. Costa RQ, Pompeu JE, Bruck SM. Avaliação da orientação espacial em um ambiente de realidade virtual em pacientes com comprometimento cognitivo leve. [thesis]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo; 2020.
18. Costa RQ, Pompeu JE, Moretto E, Silva JM, Santos MD, Nitrini R, et al. Two immersive virtual reality tasks for the assessment of spatial orientation in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: concurrent validity, group comparison, and accuracy results. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;1-13.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721000655
19. Carvalho VA, Caramelli P. Brazilian adaptation of the Addenbrookeâ?Ts Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-R). Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):212-6.
https://doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200015
20. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183-94.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x
21. Witmer BG, Singer MJ. Measuring presence in virtual environments: a presence questionnaire. Presence. 1998;7(3):225-40.
https://doi.org/10.1162/105474698565686
22. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell K, Arnold R, Hodges JR. The Addenbrookeâ?Ts Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21(11):1078-85.
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1610
23. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Baddeley A, Hiorns R. The development and validation of a test battery for detecting and monitoring everyday memory problems. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1989;11(6):855-70.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638908400940
24. Bujang MA, Baharum N. A simplified guide to determination of sample size requirements for estimating the value of intraclass correlation coefficient: a review. Arch Orofac Sci. 2017;12(1):1-11.
25. Lindenau JD, Guimarães LS. Calculando o tamanho de efeito no SPSS. Rev HCPA. 2012;32(3):363-81. ISSN: 2357-9730
26. Weech S, Kenny S, Cowan MB. Presence and Cybersickness in virtual reality are negatively related: a review. Front Psychol. 2019;10:158.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00158
27. Rebenitsch L, Owen C. Review on cybersickness in applications and visual displays. Virtual Reality. 2016;(20):101-25.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-016-0285-9
28. De Leo G, Diggs LA, Radici E, Mastaglio TW. Measuring sense of presence and user characteristics to predict effective training in an online simulated virtual environment. Simul Healthc. 2014;9(1):1-6.
https://doi.org/10.1097/SIH.0b013e3182a99dd9
29. Morganti F, Marrakchi S, Urban PP, Iannoccari GA, Riva G. A virtual reality based tool for the assessment of â?osurvey to routeâ?Tâ?T spatial organization ability in elderly population: preliminary data. Cogn Process. 2009;10 Suppl 2:S257-9.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0284-9
30. Byagowi A, Moussavi Z. Design of a virtual reality navigational (VRN) experiment for assessment of egocentric spatial cognition. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2012;4812-5.
https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2012.6347070
31. Serino S, Morganti F, Stefano FD, Riva G. Detecting early egocentric and allocentric impairments deficits in Alzheimerâ?Ts disease: an experimental study with virtual reality. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7(88).
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00088
32. Poyua OR, Byagowi A, Kelly DM, Moussavi Z. Introducing a new age-and-cognition-sensitive measurement for assessing spatial orientation using a landmark-less virtual reality navigational task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2017;70(7):1406-19.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1187181
33. Mohammadi A, Kargar M, Hesami E. Using virtual reality to distinguish subjects with multiple- but not single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment from normal elderly subjects. Psychogeriatrics. 2018;18(2):132-42.
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyg.12301
34. Tascón L, Castillo J, Cimadevilla JM. Walking and non-walking space in an equivalent virtual reality task: Sexual dimorphism and aging decline of spatial abilities. Behav Brain Res. 2018;347:201-8.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.022
This study was conducted by the Physiotherapy, Speech Therapy and Occupational Therapy Department, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo â?" São Paulo, SP, Brazil.
1. Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Departamento de Fonoaudiologia, e Terapia Ocupacional, São Paulo SP, Brazil
2. Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Departamento de Neurologia, São Paulo SP, Brazil
3. Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade Politécnica, Departamento de Engenharia, São Paulo SP, Brazil
Correspondence:
Michelle Didone dos Santos
Email:
michelle.didone@hotmail.com
Received on November 01, 2021
Accepted in final form on November 19, 2021
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Funding: This work was carried out with the support of the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento CientÃfico e Tecnológico (CNPq) â?" Brazil, through a grant to the first author. This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de NÃvel Superior â?" Brasil (CAPES) â?" Finance Code 001, through a grant to the second author. This research was conducted with support from the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP), process nos. 16/04984-3 and 14/22348-1, through scholarships granted to the third and last authors, respectively.