Vol. 6 nº 3 - Jul/Aug/Set de 2012
Original Article Pages 145 to 151
 

Dysexecutive syndrome and cerebrovascular disease in non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the literature
Síndrome disexecutiva e doença cerebrovascular no comprometimento cognitivo leve não-amnésico: uma revisão sistemática da literatura

Authors: Felipe Kenji Sudo1; Carlos Eduardo Oliveira Alves1; Gilberto Sousa Alves1; Letice Ericeira-Valente1; Chan Tiel2,3; Denise Madeira Moreira2,5; Jerson Laks1,4; Eliasz Engelhardt2,3

PDF

Descriptors: mild cognitive impairment, vascular dementia, executive function, neuropsychology, neuroimaging, cerebrovascular disease.
Descritores:
comprometimento cognitivo leve, demência vascular, função executiva, neuropsicologia, neuroimagem, doença cerebrovascular.

ABSTRACT:
OBJECTIVE: Non-amnestic dysexecutive Vascular Mild Cognitive Impairment (VaMCI) may represent preclinical Vascular Dementia (VaD). The aim of this study was to summarize the clinical, neuropsychological and neuroimaging aspects of VaMCI; and to assess its patterns of progression to dementia.
METHODS: Searches were made in the ISI Web of Knowledge, PubMed and Lilacs databases, using the terms "mild cognitive impairment" and "executive function". Altogether, 944 articles were retrieved.
RESULTS: VaMCI cases had poorer performances on fronto-executive tasks, a higher prevalence of stroke, presence of periventricular and profound white matter hyperintensities on MRI images, as well as more extrapyramidal signs and behavioral symptoms. Executive dysfunction might be associated with disconnection of fronto-parietal-subcortical circuits. Progression to dementia was associated with baseline deficits in executive function, in simple sustained attention and language, and large periventricular WMH.
DISCUSSION. VaMCI develops with impairment in non-memory domains and subcortical white matter changes on MRI images, which are consistent with clinical and neuroimaging findings in VaD.

RESUMO:
OBJETIVO: Comprometimento Cognitivo Leve Vascular (CCLV) não-amnésico disexecutivo pode representar a fase pré-clínica da Demência Vascular (DV). Este estudo tem por objetivo descrever os aspectos clínicos, cognitivos e de neuroimagem do CCLV, além de avaliar padrões de progressão para demência.
MÉTODOS: Foram realizadas buscas nas bases de dados ISI Web of Knowledge, PubMed e Lilacs, usando os termos "comprometimento cognitivo leve" e "função executiva". Ao todo, 944 artigos foram obtidos.
RESULTADOS: Pacientes com CCLV apresentaram piores desempenhos em tarefas fronto-executivas, histórico de acidentes vasculares encefálicos, presença de hiperintensidades da substância branca periventriculares e profundas, e mais, sinais extrapiramidais e sintomas comportamentais. A disfunção executiva parece se relacionar à desconexão de circuitos fronto-parieto-subcorticais. A progressão para demência relacionou-se a déficits em linguagem, função executiva e atenção sustentada à primeira avaliação, e a hiperintensidades na substância branca periventricular extensas.
DISCUSSÃO: O CCLV cursa com comprometimento cognitivo em domínios não-memória e alterações da substância branca subcortical à imagem por RM alinhadas com os achados característicos da DV.

INTRODUCTION

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) refers to an intermediate stage of cognitive decline between normal aging and dementia. Although early studies focused on the association between amnestic-subtype MCI (aMCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD),1,2 empirically-based population studies have suggested that prodromal phases of both neurodegenerative and vascular dementias might pass through non-amnestic presentations of MCI.3,4 In 2003, an international working group met in Stockholm and expanded the concept of MCI to encompass pre-dementia syndromes related to other outcomes.5 Petersen and colleagues proposed a diagnostic algorithm for identifying MCI patients and predicting possible etiologies and progression patterns, according to subtypes. This classification system differentiated four subgroups of MCI: amnestic (single and multiple domains) and non-amnestic (single and multiple domains). Non-amnestic single-domain MCI could represent preclinical Fronto-temporal Lobar Degeneration, whereas non-amnestic multidomain MCI might be at-risk for progression to Vascular Dementia (VaD).6 In fact, some recent studies using clustering techniques have demonstrated different subtypes of MCI according to impaired cognitive domain. Delano-Wood et al. reported evidence for an amnestic group, an amnestic/language group, a dysexecutive/information processing speed group, and a mixed multidomain group.7 Similarly, Libon et al. found evidence for an amnestic group and a dysexecutive group.8 The American Psychiatric Association included, in a preliminary draft for the upcoming 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), the diagnostic criteria for Minor Cognitive Disorder, which would be a predementia condition analogous to MCI related to different etiologies such as AD, Vascular Disease, Fronto-temporal Lobar Degeneration, Traumatic Brain Injury, Lewy Body Disease, Parkinson's Disease, HIV infection, Substance Abuse, Huntington's Disease and Prion Disease.9

Non-amnestic MCI (non-aMCI) corresponds to individuals with predominant impairment in one or multiple non-memory domains that is not of sufficient magnitude to meet criteria for dementia. Prevalence estimates for non-amnestic MCI range from 17 to 38% of all MCI subjects.3,10 Single-domain non-amnestic MCI individuals seem to be less likely to convert to dementia than aMCI, but a study suggested that they might have higher rates of death over 5 years.4 Dysexecutive mild cognitive impairment (dMCI) can be defined as single-domain MCI with scores at or below the 10th percentile of control performance on at least one executive function task, and scores within one standard deviation of normal means in memory assessment.11 This might be a relatively common condition with a mean prevalence of 16.77% among MCI individuals.8,12,13

There is growing evidence that MCI cases with impairment in executive function (EF) may present cerebrovascular disease (CVD). This group might exhibit greater white-matter lesion (WML) volumes in comparison to groups with MCI due to Alzheimer Disease (MCI-AD).7 A possible neuroanatomic mechanism driving dysexecutive syndrome might be the interruption of reciprocal and intimate connections between prefrontal cortex and the dorsal medial nucleus of the thalamus; in dMCI, it is possible that downwardly projecting pathways from prefrontal cortex towards the thalamus might be compromised, limiting the capacity of the prefrontal cortex to engage in its superordinate executive functions.12 Likewise, a previous study from this group showed that, compared to NCs, dMCI patients had higher severity of periventricular and profound WML as well as more depressive symptoms and lower scores on CLOX, Trail-Making Test, praxis, abstraction and CAMCOG's global score.14 Thus, individuals with MCI suffering from dysexecutive syndrome might also be associated to high-risk for progression to VaD.15

Our objectives in this systematic review were as follows: to describe clinical, neuropsychological and neuroimaging aspects of vascular-related non-amnestic dysexecutive MCI patients, and to assess patterns of progression to dementia of non-amnestic VaMCI.


METHODS

A review of the literature was performed through searches in the electronic databases PubMed (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/), Institute for Scientific Information Web of Knowledge (http://www.isiknowledge.com) and LILACS (http://lilacs.bvsalud.org/), using the terms "mild cognitive impairment" and "executive function". We also hand-searched articles cited in the selected papers, so that publications missed by the electronic research could be added.

Inclusion criteria were as follows: original articles written in English, Spanish, Portuguese or French; studies focusing on vascular-related non-amnestic dysexecutive subtypes of MCI, and papers that evaluated neuropsychological assessment of executive function in Vascular MCI (VaMCI). Posters, reviews and case-reports were excluded from this review, as were papers published prior to Petersen's 2004 revised criteria for diagnosis of MCI.6 Studies using predementia clinical constructs other than MCI were also excluded from this study.

Studies retrieved by the electronic searches were analyzed by two independent reviewers (F.K.S. and E.E.), who selected all articles relevant to this review according to the inclusion criteria.


RESULTS

Of the initially retrieved 944 articles, 17 met our criteria. A flow chart was designed to summarize the different phases of the research, as recommended by The PRISMA Work Group for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses16 (Figure 1). Characteristics of the selected studies are depicted in Tables 1, 2 and 3. Only 6 papers focused on single-domain dysexecutive MCI (dMCI), but in all of these the samples comprised patients with no significant vascular lesions on brain MRI, since authors had excluded patients who presented history of stroke7,8,12 and a Longstreth grade above 4 on MRI imaging.11,13,17 These articles were excluded from our study because of this reason. Papers focusing on dysexecutive vascular-related non-amnestic MCI were included in this study. Only one of the articles was a population-based study and the remaining 16 drew on samples from tertiary memory disorder clinics and research database. Five were longitudinal studies.


Figure 1. Flow diagram describing the systematic review of the literature on dysexecutive syndrome in vascular -related non -amnestic MCI.








Clinical features and assessment of fronto-executive functions. Individuals with VaMCI had poorer performances on frontal neuropsychological tests compared to neurodegenerative MCI in most studies.18 Among the tests used to evaluate fronto-executive functions, the Stroop Test18 and letter fluency19 discriminated VaMCI from MCI-AD. MCI-AD had better performance in processing speed compared to VaMCI, whereas the former presented better results on memory tests compared to the degenerative subtype.18 However, one study indicated that executive dysfunction might not be as consistently associated to VaMCI as episodic memory impairment is related to MCI-AD.20 Prior stroke in MCI subjects was independently associated with lower cognitive performances across all non-memory domains in comparison to NC, and scores on the TMT B and Digit Symbol Substitution were more strongly associated with history of stroke.21 The Abstract Thinking subtest from the CAMCOG distinguished VaMCI from NC in one study.22

A high prevalence of arterial hypertension, severity of WML on neuroimaging and presence of extrapyramidal motor signs were identified in non-amnestic mdMCI patients. Planning, problem solving and working memory were particularly impaired in mdMCI in comparison to sdMCI.23 Moreover, history of stroke was associated with a higher risk of developing non-aMCI than aMCI.18,21 A large multicenter study reported that MCI patients with neuropsychiatric and functional deficits might be more likely to have CVD than MCI with only cognitive impairment.24 Closing-in behavior, which is a tendency in figure copying to draw very close to or on top of the model, was identified in non-amnestic mdMCI and was associated with executive dysfunction.25

Ability to perform instrumental activities of daily living showed no association with number of impaired cognitive domains in one study.26 In spite of presenting poorer performances on EF tasks and progressing with more behavioral symptoms,27 non-aMCI may suffer less functional deficits than aMCI, according to one study.26

Neuroimaging. Age was independently and positively associated with severity of white-matter lesions on structural brain MRI imaging. In this same study, prevalence of subcortical white matter hyperintensities (WMH) corresponded to 92% of the sample of MCI individuals. Periventricular and lobar white matter, especially in frontal lobe, were the most predominant locations of subcortical WMH although their presence was neither associated with global cognitive performance (MMSE and CDR) nor with MCI subtype. Nevertheless, severity of periventricular and profound WMH might specifically contribute to deficits in EF.28

Recent data reported that executive dysfunction might be associated with location of WMH. Lesions in fronto-parietal and fronto-parietal-subcortical networks were associated with decline in EF among MCI patients. WMH in fronto-subcortical tracts did not significantly contribute to impairment in EF in one study.29 Studies using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) techniques with quantitative fractional anisotropy (FA) corroborated the importance of parietal-subcortical connections in dysexecutive syndromes. Quantitative DTI-FA mean values were lower in parietal regions and centrum semiovale in VaMCI, compared to both NC and non-vascular MCI. These findings were related to impairments in visuospatial and executive functions.30 On the other hand, disconnection of networks associated to frontal and temporal lobes might also be implicated in the pathophysiology of EF impairment in VaMCI. According to Grambaite et al., MCI patients with attentional and executive impairments might present, on DTI, increased ratio of white matter radial diffusivity/ mean diffusivity in frontal, cingulate and entorhinal regions, suggestive of regional demyelination and axonal atrophy. White-matter tract degeneration in frontal and cingulate regions, and cortical thinning in caudal middle frontal region, showed association with executive dysfunction.31

Course and progression to dementia. Among patients classified as mdMCI, 26% progressed to VaD in a 3-year follow-up, supporting a possible vascular etiology of this condition.37 Sachdev et al. identified a rate of conversion to dementia of 8% per year among patients with VaMCI.32 The main predictors of cognitive decline and progression from VaMCI to dementia listed in the studies were as follows: impaired EF and language at baseline,32 large amounts of periventricular WMH (but not profound WMH)33 and decline in simple sustained attention.34

Notably, some contradictory findings have been disclosed. Sachdev et al. reported that impaired cognitive performance at baseline might be a stronger predictor of progression to dementia than MRI measurements or intervals of cerebrovascular events.32 Additionally, large amounts of periventricular WMH were predictive of a more rapid decline in EF, especially in MCI patients with executive dysfunction at baseline according to Debette et al.33 Moreover, impairments in divided and sustained attention might be observed in the course of both non-aMCI and aMCI, but decline in simple sustained attention might be specifically related to risk for transition to dementia from non-aMCI.34


DISCUSSION

Consistent with our expectations, there is mounting evidence linking CVD with non-amnestic multidomain MCI. Subcortical WMH might be a very common finding, occurring in 92% of a sample of MCI patients.28 Accordingly, a history of stroke and presence of periventricular and profound WMH on brain MRI images were associated with impairments in non-memory cognitive domains, especially EF in MCI individuals.21 History of hypertension,23 severity of WML,23 prevalence of behavioral symptoms,24 extrapyramidal signs,23 and closing-in behavior25 number among clinical and neuroimaging aspects observed in non-amnestic mdMCI, which is coherent with the putative role of CVD implicated in the etiology of cognitive impairment in this group.

Fronto-parietal-subcortical network disconnection is deemed to be the most important underlying substrate of dysexecutive syndrome in VaMCI.29,30 Interruption of white matter fibers in frontal lobe connections, as detected by DTI-FA techniques, has been reported as a possible neuropathological mechanism for dysexecutive syndrome in patients with Binswanger Disease.35,36 Likewise, studies using DTI-FA corroborated the importance of parietal-subcortical connections in dysexecutive syndromes.29,30 Reduction of anisotropy was identified in parietal regions and centrum semiovale in VaMCI, which was related to executive dysfunction. In addition, WMH in parietal lobes negatively impacts glucose metabolism in frontal lobes in individuals with CDR=0 or 0.5, which might contribute to impairment in executive function.37

Previous studies containing factor analysis of putative EF measures identified multiple dimensions of cognitive processing, such as abstract thinking, cognitive flexibility, working memory and response inhibition to distractors. Due to this broad range of functions reflecting the complexity of prefrontal cortical-subcortical circuits, no single neuropsychological test has been identified as a "gold standard" measure for EF. Therefore, in order to avoid under-evaluation of its components, clinical assessment of EF must include a comprehensive set of tasks, as opposed to a single measure.38,39 The Wisconsin Sorting Card Test (WSCT),40 Stroop Color/Word Interference Test,41 Iowa Gambling Test,42 EXIT-25,43 CLOX,44 Verbal Fluency,45 Trail-Making Test (TMT) forms A and B46 and Porteus' mazes test47 are among the most used instruments to assess EF in studies. In this review, neuropsychological tests that proved useful to distinguish between VaMCI and MCI-AD were the Stroop test18 and letter fluency,19 while the Abstract Thinking subtest in CAMCOG distinguished VaMCI from NC.22

Baseline predictors of progression for patients with VaMCI were impairments in EF and language,32 large periventricular WMH33 and decline in simple sustained attention.34 Rate of progression to dementia was approximately 8% per year in VaMCI individuals.27,32 Coherently, a previous study prospectively assessing patients with Vascular Cognitive Impairment No-Dementia, a clinical construct analogous to VaMCI, identified a rate of 46% of conversion to dementia after 5 years of follow-up.48

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to review the main clinical, cognitive and neuroimaging features of dysexecutive non-amnestic VaMCI. Several limitations of this study should be acknowledged. Firstly, no data were available in the literature concerning vascular single-domain dMCI. One previous study conducted by Eppig et al. (2012) analyzed EF performances in dMCI as a function of time and revealed a decline in Mental Control tests and letter fluency, in comparison to aMCI and normal controls.12 Moreover, dMCI patients showed overall cognitive performance consistent with cognitive deficits seen in dementia due to subcortical pathology, as shown by intact anterograde memory and impaired EF tasks.12 Further studies are needed to determine the influence of WML on cognitive function in vascular dMCI patients. Secondly, most of the studies included in this review used convenience samples from tertiary hospitals or memory clinics. Large prospective longitudinal studies, using population-based settings are needed. Such epidemiological studies of vascular-related dysexecutive MCI would advance our understanding of the role of CVD in early stages of cognitive impairment, its predictive value for future conversion to dementia and highlight which clinical and cognitive aspects can help differentiate VaMCI among neurodegenerative disorders.

In summary, the studies discussed in this review suggested that cognitive testing, including EF evaluation and assessment of severity of WMH, might be important both as diagnostic tools and as predictors of conversion to dementia in early stages of Vascular Cognitive Impairment.

Acknowledgement. The authors would like to thank Dr. Evandro Coutinho for technical support and the Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (CNPq) for supporting Jerson Laks, who is a Researcher 2 of CNPq.


REFERENCES

1. Morris JC, Price JL. Pathologic correlates of nondemented aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early-stage Alzheimer's disease. J Mol Neurosc 2001;17:101-118.

2. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56:303-308.

3. Busse A, Hensel A, Gühne U, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller SG. Mild cognitive impairment: Longterm course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology 2006;67:2176-2185.

4. Yaffe K, Petersen RC, Lindquist K, Kramer J, Miller B. Subtype of mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia and death. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;22:312-319.

5. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment- -beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med 2004;256:240-246.

6. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 2004;256:183-194.

7. Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW, Sacco J, et al. Heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment: differences in neuropsychological profile and associated white matter lesion pathology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:906-914.

8. Libon DJ, Bondi MW, Price CC, et al. Verbal serial list learning in mild cognitive impairment: A profile analysis of interference, forgetting, and errors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011;17:905-914.

9. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 Development. Available at www.dsm5.org. Accessed March 25, 2012.

10. Palmer K, Backman L, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;16:603-611.

11. Pa J, Boxer A, Chao LL, et al. Clinical-Neuroimaging Characteristics of Dysexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment. Ann Neurol. 2009;65:414-423.

12. Eppig J, Wambach D, Nieves C, et al. Dysexecutive functioning in mild cognitive impairment: derailment in temporal gradient. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012;18:20-28.

13. Johnson JK, Pa J, Boxer AL, et al. Baseline preditors of clinical progression among patients with Dysexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;30:344-351.

14. Sudo FK, Alves CEO, Alves GS, et al. Dysexecutive mild cognitive impairment: clinical, cognitive and neuroimaging aspects. VIII Reunião dos Pesquisadores em Doença de Alzheimer - Brazilian Meeting on Alzheimer´s Disease and Related Disorders. 2011;5:36.

15. Frisoni GB, Galluzi S, Bresciani L, et al. Mild cognitive impairment with subcortical vascular features: clinical characteristics and outcome. J Neurol 2002;249:1423-32.

16. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al. The PRISMA Statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. Plos Medicine. 2009;6:1-28.

17. Chao L, Pa J, Duarte A, et al. Patterns of cerebral hypoperfusion in amnestic and dysexecutive MCI. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23:245-252.

18. Zhou A, Jia J. Different cognitive profiles between mild cognitive impairment due to cerebral small vessel disease and mild cognitive impairment of Alzheimer's disease origin. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:898-905.

19. Galluzzi S, Sheud CF, Zanetti O, et al. Distinctive clinical features of mild cognitive impairment with subcortical cerebrovascular disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2005;19:196-203.

20. Gainotti G, Ferraccioli M, Vita MG, et al. Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in MCI patients with small subcortical infarcts or hippocampal atrophy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14:611-619.

21. Knopman DS, Roberts RO, Geda YE, et al. Association of prior stroke with cognitive function and cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Arch Neurol 2009;66:614-619.

22. Sudo FK, Alves GS, Alves CE, et al. Impaired abstract thinking may discriminate between normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2010;68:179-184.

23. Brandt J, Aretouli E, Neijstrom E, et al. Selectivity of executive function deficits in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuropsychology 2009;23:607-618.

24. Halfelt JJ, Wuu J, Sollinger AB, et al. An exploration of subgroups of mild cognitive impairment based on cognitive, neuropsychiatric and functional features: analyses of data from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center. Am J Geriatr Psychiatr. 2011;19:940-950.

25. Ambron E, McIntosh RD, Finotto S, et al. Closing-in behavior in mild cognitive impairment: an executive deficit. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012;18:269-276.

26. Teng E, Becker BW, Woo E, et al. Subtle deficits in instrumental activities of daily living in subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord.2010;30:189-197.

27. Zanetti M, Ballabio C, Abbate C, et al. Mild cognitive impairment subtypes and vascular dementia in community-dwelling elderly people: a 3-year follow-up study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54:580-586.

28. Bombois S, Debette S, Delbeuck X, et al. Prevalence of subcortical vascular lesions and association with executive function in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Stroke 2007;38:2595-2597.

29. Jacobs HI, Visser PJ, Van Boxtel MPJ, et al. The association between white matter hyperintensities and executive decline in mild cognitive impairment in network dependent. Neurobiol Aging 2012;33:201.e1-201.e8.

30. Shim YS, Yoon B, Shon YM, et al. Difference of the hippocampal and white matter microalterations in MCI patients according to the severity of subcortical vascular changes: neuropsychological correlates of diffusion tensor imaging. Clin Neurol Neurosug 2008;110:552-561.

31. Grambaite R, Selnes P, Reinvang I, et al. Executive dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment is associated with changes in frontal and cingulate white-matter tracts. J Alzheimer's Disease 2011;27:453-462.

32. Sachdev PS, Chen X, Brodaty H, et al. The determinants and longitudinal course of post-stroke mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol. 2009;15:915-923.

33. Debette S, Bombois S, Bruandet A, et al. Subcortical hyperintensities are associated with cognitive decline in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Stroke 2007;38:2924-2930.

34. Saunders NL, Summers MJ. Longitudinal deficits in attention, executive and working memory in subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2011;25:237-248.

35. Engelhardt E, Moreira DM, Alves GS, et al. Binswanger's disease: quantitative fractional anisotropy. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2009;67:179-184.

36. Engelhardt E, Moreira DM, Alves GS, et al. The corpus callosum in Binswanger's disease - A quantitative fractional anisotropy analysis. Dement Neuropsychol 2008;2:278-283.

37. Tullberg M, Fletcher E, DeCarli C, et al. White-matter lesions impair frontal lobe function regardless of their location. Neurology 2004;63:246-253.

38. Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, et al. Executive control function: A review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Associations. J Neuropsych Clin Neurosc 2002;14:377-405.

39. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter. Contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 2000;41:49-100.

40. Robinson A, Heaton R, Leahman R, Stilson D. The utility of Wisconsin Card Sorting test in detecting and localizing frontal lobe lesions. J Consult Clin Psychology 1980;48:605-614.

41. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643-62.

42. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson S. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994;50:7-15.

43. Royall DR, Mahurin RK, Gray K. Bedside assessment of executive impairment: the Executive Interview (EXIT). J Am Geriatr Soc 1992; 40:1221-1226.

44. Royall DR, Cordes JA, Polk M. CLOX: an executive clock drawing task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:588-594.

45. Rosen WG. Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1980;2:135-146.

46. Reitan RM. Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271-276.

47. Porteus SD. New applications of the Porteus Maze tests. Percept Mot Skills 1968;26:787-798.

48. Wentzel C., Rockwood K, MacKnight C, et al. Progression of impairment in patients with vascular cognitive impairment without dementia. Neurology 2001;57:714-716.










1. Instituto de Psiquiatria, Center for Alzheimer's Disease (CDA/IPUB), Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil.
2. Instituto de Neurologia Deolindo Couto (INDC), Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil.
3. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, INDC-CDA/IPUB, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil.
4. Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro; 5Hospital Pró-Cardíaco, Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil.

Felipe Kenji Sudo
Rua Barata Ribeiro, 383/1004
22040-001 Rio de Janeiro RJ - Brazil
E-mail: fksudo@gmail.com

Received May 20, 2012.
Accepted in final form July 20, 2012.
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.

 

Home Contact